Topical Blog Week #8 (Due Wednesday)

| 19 Comments

What we would like you to do is to find a topic from this week's chapter that you were interested in and search the internet for material on that topic. You might, for example, find people who are doing research on the topic, you might find web pages that discuss the topic, you might find a video clip that demonstrates something related to the topic, etc. What you find and use is pretty much up to you at this point. Please be sure to use at least 3 quality resources. If you use videos, please limit it to one video.

Once you have completed your search and explorations we would like you to:
1a) State what your topic is.
1b) Discuss how the topic relates to the chapter.
1c) Discuss why you are interested in it.

2) Next, we would like you to take the information you read or viewed related to your topic, integrate/synthesize it, and then write about the topic in a knowledgeable manner. By integrating/synthesizing we mean taking what your read/experienced from the internet search organize the information into the main themes, issues, info, examples, etc. about your topic and then write about the topic in your own words using the information you have about the topic.

3) List the terms you used from the text and from your reference websites.

4) At the end of your post, please include working URLs for the three websites. For each URL you have listed indicate why you chose the site and the extent to which it contributed to your post.

19 Comments

Change blindness (CB)

Change blindness is a phenomenon in visual perception in which very large changes occurring in full view in a visual scene are not noticed. This topic is directed linked to chapter 7 in our text (pgs. 210-217)

After researching the subject of change blindness, I found that recently a number of studies have shown that under certain circumstances, very large changes can be made in a picture without people noticing them. What characterizes the experiments showing such "change blindness" in visual scenes is the fact that the changes are arranged to occur at the same time with some kind of extraneous, brief disruption in visual continuity, such as the large retinal disturbance produced by an eye saccade, a shift of the picture, a brief flicker, a "mud splash", an eye blink, or a film cut in a motion picture sequence (just examples). These phenomena are attracting an increasing amount of attention from experimental psychologists and from philosophers, because they suggest that humans' internal representation of the visual world is much smaller than they thought.

One of the first experiments on CB were done in the context of saccadic suppression by Bridgeman, Hendry & Stark (1975) and they found that large displacements of the entire visual field were not noticed if they occurred during saccadic eye movements.

That triggered interest in CB from McConkie & Currie (1996) where participants viewed visual scenes presented on a computer monitor, while their eye movements were being measured. The computer could make changes in the scene as a function of where the observer looked. For example, when the observer looked from the door of a house to the window, say, the window (or some other element of the scene such as the sky, or the car parked in front of the house) changed in some way: it could disappear, be replaced by a different element, change colour, change position, etc.

It was found that when the change occurred during an eye movement, surprisingly large changes could be made without the observers noticing them. Elements of the picture that occupied as much as a fifth of the picture area would not be seen.

At first, the explanation of the phenomenon was assumed to have something to do with the mechanisms the brain uses to combine information from successive eye fixations to form a unified view of the visual world. In particular, every time the eye moves, the retinal image shifts. Some mechanism in the brain may correct for such shifts in order to create a stable view of the world. However the mechanism could be imperfect and not take into account certain differences in the visual content across the shift, thereby explaining why changes made during saccades might sometimes go unnoticed.

From my research here are some theories of change blindness:
Currently accepted explanation of change blindness owes much to the work done in the 1960's and 70's showing how visual information is transferred via an attentional "bottleneck" to a very low capacity short term visual storage done by Gegenfurtner & Sperling in 1993.

Concluding with the explanation of change blindness involves two components: a component related to what is called "visual transients", and a component related to the way a scene is encoded in memory.

Visual transients are fast changes in luminance or color in the retinal image, such as would be produced by a sudden appearance or disappearance, or through motion of an element of the scene. It is known that such transients are detected in the first levels of the visual system, and that attention is automatically attracted to the location where they occur.
Under normal viewing conditions therefore, when a change occurs, it produces a visual transient which attracts attention to the change location. The transient thus provides information that a change has occurred, and it says where it occurred, but it does not provide information about what the change was.

In order for an observer to be able to determine what the change was, he or she will have had to have encoded into visual memory what was at the change location before the change occurred, and compare it to what is there after the change. (WOW!{me})

Lastly, and on a personal note, all this is completely and quite fascinating to me especially because I believe I suffer from change blindness. It took me a long time to recognize the differences, longer than most during my personal experiments.

http://wexler.free.fr/library/files/bridgeman%20%281975%29%20failure%20to%20detect%20displacement%20of%20the%20visual%20world%20during%20saccadic%20eye%20movements.pdf
- This website is shown to be the first real experiment linked to Change Blindness. It provided me with the knowledge and information needed to complete my blog on the topic from chapter 7.

http://www.gocognitive.net/demo/change-blindness & http://www.cogsci.uci.edu/~ddhoff/cb.html
- These two websites provided me with a real demo of the test that some psychologists use. Also, gave me actual insight of what it feels like to be a participant in an experiment.

http://www.ic.ucsc.edu/~bruceb/psyc123/ChangeBlindnesslab4.html
- This website provided me with the first resources that lead to further research on the subject of Change Blindness.

Terms: Change blindness, visual perception, psychologists, philosophers, eye, experiments, visual continuity, retinal, brain, visual field, test, resources, research, eye movement, experiment, participants, observers, perception, element, visual, color, retinal, image, subject and scene.

The topic I choose to research was simultanagnosia, which is related to Balint’s syndrome. This topic was discussed in the chapter and is related to visual attention disorders. I thought this topic was interesting because it reminded me of tunnel vision which I have heard of before, and I wanted to look more into it. In addition, I can’t imagine only being able to focus on one thing at a time in such a complex world. We spend most of our lives trying to avoid moving objects and maneuver around objects, and I can’t imagine standing in a crowd and only seeing someone’s hat, or standing in a forest and only seeing a leaf.

Simultanagnosia is the inability to see more than one object at a time in a complex scene. While it is not the only symptom or disorder related to Balint’s syndrome it is the most prominent. According to my sources it was first used in 1924 by Wolpert, who used it to describe a subject who could see details of a complex scene but was unable to describe the overall meaning of a scene. There are two categories that simultanagnosia can be divided into depending on which part of the brain the injury lies in. These two categories are dorsal and ventral. Dorsal being a lesion to the junction between the parietal and occipital lobe, while ventral is a lesion between the junction between the occipital and temporal region. These lesions can be caused by brain trauma, strokes, or some disorders such as dementia or Huntington’s disease. Typically the ventral version is less serious and damaging than the dorsal form. People with this disorder are similar to blind people in some of their behaviors such as running into objects in a room, or not seeing moving objects coming towards them or moving past them. But, it has been found that those with the ventral form of this disorder display the behavior of bumping into furniture far less often. While there are no definite ways to diagnose this disorder, typically subjects are told to observe a complex scene and try to give the overall meaning of the image (this is also called global processing). Most of the time they can describe the objects in the scene, one at a time, but cannot tell you what the overall scene is depicting. Another symptom of Balint’s syndrome include gaze apraxia which is the inability to shift one’s gaze to a new stimuli. Some experiments have found that the nature of an object may determine how well it can be seen or identified. They found that when letters are put into words, subjects are able to see more of the letters, even if they can’t tell you what they mean. They were also able to see shapes of one color better than shapes that were multi-colored. While I did not find a lot of information on the types of treatments available for this disorder I found that occupational therapy is sometimes used. In addition a lot of the same services that blind people are provided can be used to help those with Balint’s syndrome or simultanagnosia. These services may include books on tape, and the development of other senses to compensate for poor vision. Often times this disorder is missed or mis-diagnosed because it is so rare and because diagnosing tools are limited.

Terms Used: balint’s syndrome, simultanagnosia, visual attention, complex scene, dorsal, ventral, lesion, parietal and occipital lobe, temporal region, global processing, gaze apraxia, occupational therapy

http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Simultanagnosia
I used this source as a jumping off point for the content in my blog. It provided me with an overall knowledge of the topic. I used it quite a bit in the development of this blog.


https://books.google.com/books?id=eCXgtVIsUYkC&pg=PA145&lpg=PA145&dq=simultagnosia&source=bl&ots=vuc0DbzqS6&sig=qF4fH8fYMLz5AAjJfOLJDo8KdV0&hl=en&sa=X&ei=UWP2VMrQOIv8yQTZ5YIQ&ved=0CFEQ6AEwCTgK#v=onepage&q=simultagnosia&f=false
this source was book and was very informative. It had a lot of really nice images of the brain to go along with the topic. it reinforced some content from the other two sources but also offered new content.

http://neurology.about.com/od/Stroke/a/What-Is-Balint-Syndrome.htm
this source I used the least but it was the one of the only sources I found that really stated anything about treatment options for those with this disorder, so it was really helpful in that sense. I find that my last source I use the least because it is typically only reinforcing the other sites.

1a) State what your topic is.
My topic is visual-field defect.

1b) Discuss how the topic relates to the chapter.

Visual-field defect was talked about in Chapter 7. Visual-field defect is a portion of the visual field with no vision or with abnormal vision, typically resulting from damage to the visual nervous system. Brain damage that produces this deficit is actually pretty rare. A person who is unfortunate enough to lose this primary visual cortex in the right hemisphere will be blind on the opposite side of the visual space.

1c) Discuss why you are interested in it.

The reason I’m interested in this is because I think it’s a really sad but interesting problem. Not a lot caught my eye this chapter but I did think visual-field defect was interesting. I really had no idea that there was another visual problem that was so closely linked to blindness. I’m sure someone going through this after brain damage is at least thankful they can somewhat see. But I’m sure it’s still something extremely hard to deal with at the same time. I wanted to research more into how much someone with this can see, or what exactly they do see.

The visual field is the portion of the subject’s surroundings that can be seen at any one time. The normal extent of field of vision is 50 degrees superiorly, 60 nasally, 70 inferiorly, and 90 temporarily. A visual field defect is a loss of part of the usual field of vision, so it does not include blindness of either eye or both eyes. The lesion may be anywhere along the optic pathway; retina to occipital cortex.

Visual field can be lost due to age-related macular degeneration, optic neuropathy, leber’s optic atrophy, macular holes, cone dystrophies, and different conditions like Best’s disease, Stargardt’s disease, and achromatopsia. Peripheral field loss occurs with glaucoma, retinal detachment, retinitis pigmentosa, and chorioretinitis.

Visual acuity tests the eye’s greatest power of resolution whereas visual field testing measures the peripheral sensitivity. The image is projected on to the retina upside down and inverted. A lesion on the top right of the retina or the pathway beyond will cause a defect in the bottom left of the visual field. Assessing for visual field defects can be looked at during screening tests which include confrontational visual field testing or with the use of an Amsler grid. Or with quantitative measurements using manual or automated perimetry.

Scotoma is another related visual field defect. It is a defect surrounded by normal visual field. Relative scotoma is an area where objects of low luminance cannot be seen but larger or brighter ones can. Absolute scotoma is where nothing can be seen at all within the area. Hemianopia is a binocular visual defect in each eye’s hemifield. Bitemporal hemianopia is where the two halves lost are on the outside of each eye’s peripheral vision, effectively creating central visual tunnel.


Homonymous hemianopia is where the two halves lost are on the corresponding area of the visual field in both eyes, either the left or the right half of the visual field. Altitudinal hemianopia is the dividing line between loss and sight being horizontal rather than vertical with visual loss either above or below the line. Quadrantanopia is an incomplete hemianopia referring to a quarter of the visual field loss. Sectoral defect is also an incomplete hemianopia.

Confrontation visual field testing is the main method of assessing visual field loss. It is a qualitative measurement but is the best starting point. Traditionally a hatpin has been used to define the visual field. A red or white head is used and it may be moved across the visual field to ascertain where it disappears and hence to define a scotoma. The amsler grid testing is also a good testing method. This asses the central 10 degree visual field. It detects central and paracentral scotomas.

Retinal detachment is related to visual-field defects. Retinal detachment and occlusion of blood vessels at a level smaller than the central retinal give defects with boundaries in the horizontal meridian. It tends to be fairly rapid in onset. It may follow trauma or there may be predisposing factors. It may be preceded by floaters and flashes before what the person may describe as a curtain coming across the visual field. A crescentric red or orange slip or detachment may be apparent at the periphery of the retina. Central retinal artery occlusion tends to be a sudden and complete loss of vision in one eye but if the occlusion is at the level of one of the four arteries to the retina, there will be loss of just a quadrant of field.

Terms: visual-field defect, nervous system, visual, vision, abnormal vision, primary visual cortex, blind, optic pathway, retina, occipital cortex, macular degeneration, optic neuropathy, leber’s optic atrophy, macular holes, cone dystrophies, acuity tests, resolution, peripheral, Amsler grid, perimetry, relative scotoma, luminance, absolute scotoma, hemianopia, quadrantanopia, confrontational visual field testing, retinal detachment, horizontal meridian.

URL: http://www.patient.co.uk/doctor/visual-field-defects

URL: http://almostadoctor.co.uk/content/systems/ophthalmology/visual-field-defects

URL: http://www.britannica.com/EBchecked/topic/630842/visual-field-defect

The reason I chose these websites is because they talked about the causes and effects of visual-field defects. They all gave me a better understanding of it. The second website gave me a really good diagram of the eyes and how the defects are different from “normal” eyes.

1. The topic I chose was visual neglect. This relates to the chapter because it is a disorder that results in the inability to attend to objects in a portion of the field of vision. This connects directly to the chapter because the chapter focuses on the methods of attention that we use. I found this interesting because my general interests revolve around disorders of the brain. I also find it incredibly fascinating that someone with properly working eyes and optical centers could be basically rendered blind on one side, simply due to the inability to focus attention. It truly shows how much our senses rely on our ability to attend to the stimuli presented to us.

2. Visual neglect, or hemispatial neglect is an attention disorder of the brain. Those who have the disorder are unable to focus their attention on the contralesional side of the brain. The term contralesional refers to the side on the opposite of the brain where the problem is located. The disorder mainly occurs due to the damage of the parietal lobe of the brain. This damage could occur in the form of a stroke or another type of brain trauma. Typically, hemispatial neglect is caused by damage to the right parietal lobe. This causes the left field of vision to be the neglected side, and the individual to direct their attention to the ipsilesional side (the same side as the brain damage). It is believed that visual neglect occurs this way a majority of the time because of the specialties of each side of the brain. While it is thought that the left side of the brain deals mostly with language, the right side is believed to deal with spatial memory. This difference shows why hemispatial neglect is mostly caused by damage to the right side of the brain. When speaking of neglect in this disorder, one speaks of the “neglect” of the senses on that side of the body. While this neglect is most visible in vision, some patients have decreased hearing ability, as well as motor neglect. Motor neglect is the tendency not to use a body part, which does not display muscle weakness. Therefore, it goes without saying that hemispatial neglect can be dangerous even in mild cases when one cannot effectively see half the world.
Severe cases of hemispatial neglect are often easily spotted. They may read a book and only pay attention to the right-side pages. They may also not realize if someone is trying to talk to them on their left side. Typically, symptoms are not as easy to define as neglect. However, the trained eye could piece together these following symptoms to make a diagnosis. A person with this disorder could only shave or apply makeup to one side of the face. They could also run into objects on the left side of their field of vision, ignorant of the object’s existence. Hemispatial neglect is often diagnosed through a series of tests called the Behavioral Inattention Test (BIT). This is composed of six subtests. One of the most well known of these is for the patient to draw a daisy presented to them. Those with the disorder will only draw one half of the daisy.
Hemispatial neglect is often confused with another disorder called Hemianopsia. Hemianopsia is vision disorder that damages the field of vision on either the left or right side. This means that a person looking at a tree would only see one side of the tree, depending on if they had left or right hemianopsia (right means the right side is missing and left means the left side is missing). If the disorder occurs in both eyes, it is called homonymous hemianopsia. This is different from hemianopsia because this is a disorder of vision, not attention. That being said, it is very possible to have both disorders at the same time.
Another main difference between hemispatial neglect and hemianopsia is that hemispatial neglect can most often be paired with anosognosia. Anosognosia is the inability to recognize that there is a problem with the patient’s field of vision. Because of this, many with this disorder are not aware there is a problem. They may go so far as to deny the problem’s existence when it is presented to them. Those with hemianopsia are most often very aware that there is a problem. This makes hemianopsia the less severe of the two because patients are able to compensate for the loss of vision field by simply turning their head. A person with neglect may be able to do this, but they would need to be aware of the problem first. A very famous example of anasognosia was President Woodrow Wilson. After suffering two strokes, he developed hemispatial neglect as well as homogynous left hemianopsia. He would very often not acknowledge persons who engaged him on his left side. He would only recognize they were there when they were moved to his right side. His defiance of the disorder extended so far as he had planned to run a third presidential campaign until his wife put her foot down.
This disorder can sometimes show improvement within a few weeks of the initial damage, but many show persistant symptoms. There are treatments out there, yet they are often not incredibly effective. However, they are slowly making ground. Therefore, the outlook for this disorder is hopeful, yet still a bit bleak.

http://jnnp.bmj.com/content/75/1/13.full
I liked this source because it was a very in depth article that provided a great deal of information that the other sources tended to skim over. I used this source for the added knowledge.

https://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/brain-babble/201208/hemispatial-neglect-one-sided-world
I liked this article because it was a very easy to read basis for the information I was searching. Through this article, I was able to gain a basic understanding of this disorder. After I knew the basics, I could move on to more in-depth material.

http://www.hemianopsia.net/visual-neglect/
I like this source because it was a slightly more in depth article that provided easy to read information. Along with this it went into more detail than the basic article I used.

Terms: visual neglect; hemispatial neglect; attention disorder; parietal; contralesional; anosognosia; motor neglect; homonymous hemianopsia; hemianopsia; field of vision; ipsilesional

The topic I chose to further research was change blindness. Change blindness relates to chapter seven in many different ways. Chapter seven talks about the visual system and the way we notice and see things that grab our attention. Chapter seven also talks about how at times our eyes fail to notice change in different scenes. Change blindness is that failure to notice a change between two scenes. I find this topic interesting because I always thought I was good at picking out the differences between two scenes, meaning that I always notice the small details that changed. After reading about change blindness in the book, I wanted to further research it. Sensation and perception has got me interested in so many different subjects but I believe that change blindness is one of the more interesting subjects I have researched.

According to Wikipedia, “change blindness is a surprising perceptual phenomenon that occurs when a change in a visual stimulus is introduced and the observer does not notice it.” This is close to what I defined change blindness as before but I like how this definition uses the particular word ‘phenomenon’. To me, all things are a phenomenon. How is it that some things are understood but some things we just can’t seem to figure out? Research on change blindness developed from interest in the phenomena of eye movements and working memory. Sometimes people are really good at remembering things but when it comes to recalling the smaller details it automatically becomes a challenge. Sometimes change blindness can be confused with inattentional blindness. Inattentional blindness is defined as an individual failing to recognize an unexpected stimulus in plain sight. Although both of these terms involve failing to recognize something, change blindness involves two separate scenes changing and inattentional blindness involves one scene adding a stimulus.

There are a few factors that I want to talk about that influence change blindness. Age is one of these factors and there have been studies over this particular influence. These studies concluded that older individuals were slower to detect the changes in an experiment than younger individuals who took the same test. This is also true when different age groups were asked to make a decision of a change blindness paradigm used at an intersection. The older individuals failed to make the right decision more often than the younger individuals. Another factor influencing change blindness is object presentation. This is the way in which objects appear. Even when there is not a delay between the original image or scene and the altered image or scene change blindness can occur but only if the change forces the viewer to redefine the objects. Both the appearance of a new object and the forthcoming of an object are more resistant to change blindness than the withdrawing of an object.

There are also some practical implications relating to the phenomenon of change blindness. Eyewitness testimony and the inaccuracy of it could have something to do with change blindness. Witnesses are rarely able to detect a change in the criminal’s identity unless first intending to remember the incident in question which in turn can lead to inaccuracy in identifying these criminals. In court, change blindness is starting to be questioned so we can weed out mistaken eyewitness identification and wrongful conviction.
Driving ability is another practical implication that relates to change blindness. Like I stated earlier, older individuals are slower to detect changes in scenes than younger individuals. When you are driving, the location and relevance of the changes happening can have an effect on what is noticed while driving. We can obviously see better and have a faster reaction time when we are in the center of our visual field instead of using our peripherals. This leads to the conclusion that drivers are able to recognize relevant changes more so than irrelevant changes. Researching the effects of change blindness could lead to answering some questions as to why car accidents happen. When I was reading about this I was thinking to myself about how cool different things we study relate to such intense and interesting things like these.

Change blindness is an interesting subject and sometimes hard to study because how do we know that the answers we are coming up with are accurate? Sometimes past experiences and expectations can have an effect on the way that we see things from scene to scene. Some people know that changes that occur in real life are usually easy to see so it is almost like they expected the change which makes it easier to detect. When we know that something is about to happen that means it has our attention. When something has our attention, we are more apt to notice things relative to that situation rather than something that doesn’t have your attention at that time.

There have been different studies done on change blindness and there was one particular study done by Simons and Levin (1998) that stood out to me involving a human being as the changing stimulus. The participants involved had to start having a conversation with a stranger. After so long, the stranger was replaced with a different stranger during a brief interruption of a large object coming between them. What I found interesting was that the participants did not realize that the stranger they had been conversing with had changed. I found this study interesting because this leads into the ability to recognize faces also known as prosopagnosia. We studied this in previous chapters so I found it interesting how this can relate back to that.
Terms: Change blindness, sensation, perception, inattentional blindness, attention, visual system, visual field.

http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Change_blindness - I like to use Wikipedia because there is usually a ton of information on it that helps me better understand the subject that I am researching. There was a ton of information on change blindness and it was interesting to read about it.

http://psychology.about.com/od/cognitivepsychology/f/change-blindness.htm - I liked this source because it shared a lot of research studies that have been done and it was interesting to read about them. Reading about these made me want to research more studies that have been done.

http://transcendsense.com/change-blindness-and-inattentional-blindness/ - I liked this source for research because it included a helpful video. Sometimes when I am told something rather than reading it, the information clicks a little better.

This week for my topical blog I have chosen to look further into Balint’s syndrome. This visual disorder was discussed briefly in the chapter but left me wondering about much more. I found this topic interesting because it made me think about how we look out in front of us and are able to reach for an object but what if that object was no longer visually available how would we function.

Balint syndrome is an uncommon and incompletely understood triad of severe neuropsychological impairments.Balint’s syndrome is a combination of three symptoms the first is oculomotor apraxia. This means that the victim is unable to intentionally move their eyes towards an object. Second symptom is optic ataxia which is the inability to accurately reach for something your looking at. Lastly visual simultagnosia is the inability to take in a entirety of a picture while only seeing parts. Normally balint's syndrome is a result from damage to both parietal lobes. Someone with Balint’s syndrome can’t judge her location relative to the items around her and must depend on her other senses to guide her.

Balint’s syndrome occurs most often with an acute onset as a consequence of two or more strokes. This which causes it to less likely to occur very often. Even more rarely cases of progressive balint’s syndrome have been found in degenerative disorders such as alzheimer's and other traumatic brain injuries. Some of the common symptoms can be quite debilitating since they impact the visuospatial skills, and visual scanning and attentional mechanisms. Since it has impairment of both visual and language functions it can have affect on the patient’s safety and ones home environment.

The literature is extremely spares but according to one study rehabilitation training should focus on improvement of visual scanning. There have been very few treatment strategies have been proposed and those who have have been criticized as being poorly developed and evaluated. After watching a video of someone who suffers with balint’s syndrome I feel that it would be personally scarey to go through life and have the issue of not being able to tell or know what is around them. I feel like this would cause many safety concerns for one to live on their own. I also found it very interesting to see that the man was able to reach the object with one hand but not the other. He was also not able to focus on more than one object.

http://neurology.about.com/od/Stroke/a/What-Is-Balint-Syndrome.htm- this webpage was more confusing at first because it talks as if you already know about the visual disorder but throughout reading my other sources I found that this one became very useful in explaining the different symptoms.

http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/B%C3%A1lint%27s_syndrome this webpage gave more specific information about the symptopms and treatment options about balint syndrome It also gave me a broader topic to look at when learing about the main topic

http://youtu.be/4odhSq46vtU gave me a real life situation to look at and how this man’s life is effected.

terms: balint’s syndrome, visual scanning, degenerative disorders, visuospatial skills, oculomotor apraxia, optic ataxia, visual simultagnosia,

In chapter seven, we learn several curious phenomena surrounding attention and scene perception. These topics are closely related because many times our perception depends on where our attention has been focused. To review, inattentional blindness is a failure to notice unexpected events in the scene. This is particularly likely to occur when the viewer is engaged in a difficult monitoring task, if the viewer is not particularly attending the unexpected event, or if the unexpected event is not sufficiently similar to the attended subject. For example, in Simons and Chabris’ well-known ‘gorilla-suit’ study (1999), basketball players were dressed in black and white, and participants were asked to count how many times the ball was moved on the floor, which we call a monitoring task. Here, the players are the attended subject. In separate groups, Simons and Chabris introduced an unexpected event in the form of a person in a gorilla suit or a woman in plain pale clothing and an umbrella. Overall, participants were more likely to notice the woman with the umbrella than the guy in the gorilla suit. However, when attending players dressed in black, participants were more likely to notice the guy in the gorilla suit. When attending players in white, no difference was seen between the gorilla and the woman. We can conclude that attending to the black jerseys had participants on the lookout for individuals dressed in black, which made them more likely to notice the gorilla. These findings indicate similarity to the attended object, in this case color, and facilitates noticing the unexpected event.
An interesting paradox of the visual system is that we sometimes do notice unexpected events. This is explained by the von Restorff effect. The von Restorff effect contradicts inattentional blindness by saying we are more likely to remember unexpected events. This paradox has been the subject of many studies since Simons and Chabris’ gorilla suit study. In subsequent studies, researchers have built on Simons and Chabris’ original study of inattentional blindness, determining stimuli are more likely to be noticed and remembered if they are extremely arousing. Arousal influenced memory to a higher degree than did distinctiveness. This is an interesting finding; to improve memory, objects or words should be arousing. In the context of class, conversation, or almost anything in real life, we will be more likely to attend and remember if the object of our attention is arousing. Memory also depends a great deal on context. When using a comparison list, the isolation effect works as a combination function of two factors, distinctiveness and relationship of the target to other words in the list. This is because our attentional resources are primarily consumed with finding the target. When the target is sufficiently different from the rest of the items in the list, it is more easily identified. Therefore, leftover attentional resources can be utilized for encoding.
Given the findings of these studies, we can infer the paradox is really only superficial. Subsequent to the gorilla-suit study (1999), visual studies of inattentional blindness have only expanded on the phenomena behind inattentional blindness. Many factors contribute to attention or memory of the attended object which interact in complex ways. If we could determine the arousal and distinctiveness of the target in its context, we would have better chances of predicting inattentional blindness. However, these factors are subjective and somewhat difficult to measure. At present, not much research exists (or none that I could find) to examine these.
Inattentional blindness, attention, scene perception, monitoring task, von Restorff effect, encoding

The original basketball/gorilla study reported in the chapter:
http://www.drjoebio.com/uploads/1/8/1/3/1813500/gorrila_in_our_midst.pdf
Explores factors that contribute to inattentional blindness, including difficulty of monitoring task and similarity to subject of focus, among others.
http://eds.a.ebscohost.com/eds/pdfviewer/pdfviewer?sid=e502bd48-ef8f-46cd-bc31-fb4e8adeda79%40sessionmgr4004&vid=12&hid=4111
Inattentional Blindness Von Restorff Paradox
http://eds.a.ebscohost.com/eds/pdfviewer/pdfviewer?vid=3&sid=e502bd48-ef8f-46cd-bc31-fb4e8adeda79%40sessionmgr4004&hid=4111

1a. The concept that I decided to research after reading chapter twelve is the area of the brain known as the fusiform face area. This particular area in the ventral temporal lobe of the brain has been discovered to play a large role in our ability to recognize human faces. Researchers have used incredible techniques such as functional magnetic resonance imaging to locate this specific part of the brain that becomes active when an individual views the face of someone that they recognize. This research has important implications for the social interactions that we have every day.
b. This topic relates to chapter seven because it is an important finding that relates to the physiological basis of attention and more specifically the ability specifically its ability to enhance the processing of a specific type of stimulus. the FFA (fusiform face area) also builds off of information given to us in previous chapters because it involves parts of the brain important for visual processing as well as the technology of an fMRI to indicate which areas of the brain are active when viewing a face that we recognize. This chapter was easy to understand because of my knowledge of these concepts from previous chapters.
c. I am interested in this concept because it is based off of neuroscience and the importance of a specific area in the brain associated with our ability to recognize faces. I think it is incredible that a part of the brain could be associated with recognizing every face that we see in social environments. I am also interested in the fMRI technique utilized to identify the area of the brain active when viewing faces that we recognize because it gives a researcher actual images of the brain parts receiving the most oxygenated blood to assess what areas of the brain are associated with the task at hand.
2. Kanwisher, McDermott, and Chun found that the use of an fMRI suggested that the fusiform gyrus was significantly more active when when subjects viewed faces as opposed to common objects. This general finding was then compared to other subject tests aimed at determining whether the fusiform face area reacted stronger to whole faces than non human objects and other body parts. The findings suggested that the fusiform face area responded strongest to faces that we intact and did not respond strongly to images of houses, other body parts such as a hand, and images of faces that were not intact. This research provides great evidence for the belief that the fusiform area is a major contributor to our facial recognition ability. It also rules out the idea that this area could be responsive to any human body part. I think it is also of interest that this part of the brain did not react to non human objects because it further supports the idea that there is something special about the human face that this specific brain area responds to. Another article by Gauthier et al. found that expertise in recognizing novel objects increased the functioning of the fusiform area. this is of interest because it provides evidence that the ability to recognize something with rare qualities is also helpful for recognizing individual human faces. I believe that this is a great finding because humans are able to recognize unique aspects of every person that we meet and this aids in our ability to generally recognize them in any social setting. I also think it is worth noting that this recognition of novel objects results in increased activation of the fusiform face area because the FFA is not utilized in the recognition of non human objects. I hypothesize that this ability aids in recognizing specific features of human faces rather than the whole face. Lastly, a research study by Thommes et al. found that individuals with autism spectrum disorder have a decreased interest and an impairment while processing human faces. Their study utilized an fMRI and consistently showed less activation in the fusiform gyrus area of the autism spectrum patients while viewing human faces. Researchers also found reduced reaction in the primary visual cortex of the occipital lobe that connected with the fusiform area. These deficits support the conclusion that the decreased response shown by autism spectrum disorder participants may explain their reduced interest in faces when placed in social situations. I found this to be extremely thought provoking because it shows a link between decreased activation of the fusiform area and the occurrence of autism that can account for typical characteristics of autistic individuals such as a lack of interest in faces and eye contact. Findings like this are particularly attention grabbing to me because they relate the part of the brain associated with recognizing other humans with disorders that are found widely throughout society. The connection found between these two occurrences can be replicated and expanded off of to provide new information about disorders and information possible for developing a treatment.
Terms: fusiform face area, ventral temporal lobe, functional magnetic resonance imaging, primary visual cortex, fusiform gyrus, autism spectrum disorder, occipital lobe

http://www.jneurosci.org/content/17/11/4302.full I used this article because it gave an empirically supported definition and explanation of the fusiform face area. It explained the main findings that suggest this part of the brain is used for facial recognition

http://www.nature.com/neuro/journal/v2/n6/full/nn0699_568.html I chose to use this article because it gave great information about the fusiform area and other perceptual cues that can increasingly activate this area of the brain.

http://web.a.ebscohost.com.proxy.lib.uni.edu/ehost/detail/detail?vid=8&sid=403b7112-d993-402a-97de-5f4374cbd679%40sessionmgr4001&hid=4212&bdata=JnNpdGU9ZWhvc3QtbGl2ZQ%3d%3d#db=psyh&AN=2014-22487-001 I utilized this article because it is incredibly interesting to read about a link between decreased activation in the fusiform gyrus and autism spectrum disorder. It applies this specialized area of the brain to a disorder that is experienced by millions of families around the world.

The topic I chose to do further research on was Balint's syndrome. This relates to the chapter, because it is perceptual disorder that is discussed briefly in the chapter. I found it interesting, because I can't imagine what it would be like to live with such a disorder.
Balint's syndrome is an uncommon trio of severe neuropsychological impairments that are not entirely understood. These three are the inability to perceive the visual field as a whole (simultanagnosia), difficulty in fixating the eyes (oculomotor apraxia), and the inability to move the hand to a specific object by using vision (optic ataxia). This syndrome occurs mostly as an acute onset after two or more strokes that happen in about the same place in the brain. The fact that the strokes must happen in about the same place within the brain is what makes this such a rare disorder. Someone suffering from Balint's Syndrome are not able to perceive and focus on more than one object at a time. When a spoon is held up for the patient to see, they can usually identify it as a spoon. However, when a second object, like a comb, is introduced next to the spoon, the patient will either not see the comb or will no longer see the spoon and only the comb. As the two items are moved back and forth in space, the patient will tend to go back and forth on which one they can see, but they are never able to see both at the same time. When this was done with one particular individual and he had been focusing on the two objects for long enough, he no longer saw either one and could only see the blackboard that was several feet behind in the background. When this individual was presented with just the spoon and asked to reach out and touch it, he had a great deal of difficulty. He would often try to grab several inches to the side of where the spoon actually was, almost as if he had double vision. When the spoon was shaken, the patient was able to grab it more easily, but still with some degree of difficulty. However, as soon as the spoon was held still again, he reverted to not being able to grab it. Partly because of its symptoms and partly due to its rarity, Balint's Syndrome is often misdiagnosed as blindness, psychosis, or dementia. The symptoms are generally first noticed by the therapists providing rehabilitation following brain lesions. Because most practitioners are not familiar with the disorder, the symptoms are often mistaken for something else, such as the previously mentioned misdiagnoses. Treatment for Balint's Syndrome are very few in number, and a large percentage of those few that exist are criticized as being poorly developed and evaluated. There are currently three main approaches to rehabilitation for perceptual deficits like Balint's Syndrome. The first is the adaptive, or functional, approach. This involves functional tasks utilizing the patient's strengths and abilities, helping them to compensate for problems or altering the environment to lessen their disabilities. Of the three, this is the most popular rehabilitation approach. The second strategy is the remedial approach. This involves restoration of the damaged central nervous system by training in perceptual skills, which may be generalized across all activities of daily living. This could be achieved through things such as tabletop activities or sensorimotor exercises. The third approach is the multicontext approach. This is based on the fact that learning is not automatically transferred from one situation to another. It involves practicing of a targeted strategy in multiple environments with varied tasks and movement demands, and it also incorporates self-awareness tasks. There are some, however, that question the validity of Balint's Syndrome as a whole. They say that the components in the syndrome's trio of symptoms each may represent a variety of combined defects. "Because Balint's Syndrome is not common and is difficult to assess with standard clinical tools, the literature is dominated by case reports and confounded by case selection bias, non-uniform application of operational definitions, inadequate study of basic vision, poor lesion localization, and failure to distinguish between deficits in the acute and chronic phases of recovery."

https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=4odhSq46vtU - I used this source, because it showed a visual example of a patient suffering from Balint's Syndrome and is difficulty in object perception.

http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/B%C3%A1lint's_syndrome - I used this source, because it provided a strong base of information on the topic.

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC1737727/ - I used this source, because it was a more specific publication about the possible causes of the disorder, and it was from a very reputable source.

Terms: Balint's Syndrome, Neuropsychological Impairment, Simultanagnosia, Oculomotor Apraxia, Optic Ataxia, Perceive, Adaptive, Central Nervous System, Perceptual Skills, Sensorimotor, Remedial, Multicontext

1a) State what your topic is.
In this chapter i chose to learn more about Simultagnosia, also know as the Balint Syndrome.

1b) Discuss how the topic relates to the chapter.
this topic relates to this chapter in many ways, this chapter tells us about the vital aspect of perception, the input and the output of senses, also the visual attention, it refers to set of selective mechanisms that enable to focus on some stimuli at the expense of other attention mechanisms that exist in our sensory domains, this also tells us about how we can have direct attention to just one item at a time or have multiple things that we can select to pay attention to visually at once, attention also varies over time.

1c) Discuss why you are interested in it.
i'm interested in this particular topic because i want to understand the syndrome, i myself i wear glasses because i am a near sighted individual,when i don't have my glasses on, it's very hard for me to recognize things, people, letters from a far distance, it's a struggle itself, for balint syndrome, i want to understand how they cope with it, how they adjust with it, and how those people visual can improve or get helped in away where they don't have much trouble into separating items.

2) Next, we would like you to take the information you read
as for one of my sources, the video had some people who were asked to recognize items and link them why those items might be where they're at, for example, if someone who has the Balint Syndrome was asked what a tooth paste or tooth brush is, they'd tell you exactly what it is, but the problem is that they might not understand why it would be in the bathroom, they mechanism is to replace that missing gap with something else, so in this case, they refer to their daily common knowledge, another example is couple of girls were asked to identify things from a what seem to be a salad bar, they were able to name all the things from there but they really couldn't understand why those things were there, when they were ask why do they think those things were there, they said they didn't know, but it might be because there multanagnosia can be divided into two different types: dorsal and ventral, with each taking its name from the dorsal and ventral circuits concerned with the perception of objects’ shapes and locations, respectively. These two forms of simultanagnosia are associated with different symptoms as well as damage to separate areas of the brain.
Dorsal simultanagnosia results from bilateral lesions to the junction between the parietal and occipital lobes Here, perception is limited to a single object without awareness of the presence of other stimuli. Thus, being able to see only one object at a time, a patient may collide with various objects in a room being unaware of them. Additionally, objects in motion appear more difficult to perceive.
Ventral simultanagnosia results from damage to the left inferior occipito-temporal junction.Ventral simultanagnosic patients are able to see several objects at once, but their recognition of objects is piecemeal, or limited to one object at a time. Thus, individuals with ventral simultanagnosic symptoms are capable of navigating through a room without bumping into furniture.is some healthy convention that's going on.
other topics that i found which are related to simultagnosia are
my other source also had some visual images of where is effect by the balint syndrome, your left parietal of the cortex is affected, your left primary visual cortex is also affected and your temporary cortex is affected as well.
overall, this is a serious thing, it could stop you from doing many things and could even put you in danger because your visual attention is something that you need to survive, it's easily for people who have the Balint Syndrome to mistake common things that would be easy to a normal individual often, it's something that optometrists should do more research on it and how they can improve upon it.

https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=EMor2X7PMV4
This is a video of students, giving examples of how simultgnosia affects people
http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Simultanagnosia
This source is also explaining how people deal with this kind of syndrome daily, what the diagnose for it is, and how we can help those people
http://www.neurologie.uni-duesseldorf.de/HBM99/cd/attention/2197.html
This also is a good source to look at because it has visual drawings on what is missing when it comes to someone who has simultgnosia.

TERMS: Visual Cortex, Left parietal cortex , temporary cortex, Balint Syndrome, Simultanagnosia , left primary cortex ,bilateral, ventral simultanagnosia.

1. A) Visual field defect
B) Visual field defect talks about how a this deficiency affects how much a person is able to see in their normal field of view.
C) I was interested to find out if there were treatments, but there were not many. I was surprised to find out that a person will eventually get used to this.
2. Visual field defect is a blind spot or area in the field of vision in either one of both of the eyes. It is permanent but in few cases it can be temporary or shifting. Sometimes it can also occur every so often. Usually that will only happen if a person is experiencing a migraine. Visual field defect can be central of peripheral. If it is central then it would be an optic disc of nerve problem. If it peripheral then it is along the visual pathways from the optic chiasm.
If a person if being tested then it is important to test both of the eyes separately because both of the eyes may overlap and the visual field defect may not be found. When a person is tested there will be many questions about the history of the defect. These questions include where is the field of loss, did it happen suddenly or slowly, does it affect both eyes or just one, and is it a black spot or a blur? There are many ways to a assess for visual field defect. A person could do a screening test, quantitative measurements, amsler grid testing, or in a GP surgery.
There have been multiple causes found. These causes are glaucoma, vascular disease, tomours, retinal disease, hereditary disease, optic neuritis, inflammatory processes, nutritional deficiencies, toxins, drugs, or damage to the retina. Certain patterns can help establish the underlying cause. Generalized constriction can be caused from glaucoma, retinitis pigmentosa, nysteria, or chronic high intracranial pressure. Causes of central field loss can be caused by age-related mascular degeneration, optic neuropathy, Leber's optic atrophy, mascular holes, and cone dytrophies. Causes of peripheral field loss can be from glaucoma, retinal detachment, retinis pigmentosa, or chorioretinitis.
For treatment a doctor can only help so much depending on how the person got visual field defect. There is an oral medication. The person will eventually become used to this but can only perform certain tasks. Fortunatly they can make up the visual loss by the way they look at things.
Terms: Visual field defect, glaucoma, retina, generalized constriction, retinis pigmentosa, nysteria, central field,optic neuropathy, peripheral field, optic chiasm.
http://www.britannica.com/EBchecked/topic/630842/visual-field-defect
This website discusses in great detail the variety of different causes.
http://www.patient.co.uk/doctor/visual-field-defects
This website discusses and helpful explaining the testing and assessments for visual field defect.
http://www.newhealthguide.org/Visual-Field-Defects.html
This website was helpful because it explained the different treatments, though there were not many.

I choose to do my blog topic from chapter 7 over change blindness. The book (page 213) refers to this as; the failure to notice a change between two scenes, if the gist, or meaning, of the scene is not altered, quite large changes can pass unnoticed. The book relates change blindness to Rensink's experiment: participants took several seconds on average to find the changes in certain pictures and some never managed to find the changes in similar pictures at all. You can test yourself for change blindness by taking a quiz online http://www.gocognitive.net/demo/change-blindness which I did. I quickly found that it is extremely hard to tell he difference between the pictures flashing quickly for me. The pictures show for about a second each and then flash to the next picture and you must click what is different for in the two pictures. Unless I noticed what was different right away I couldn't seem to find it at all; this was actually a very frustrating task for me. I found a basic definition online for change blindness that states •"The term 'change blindness' refers to the surprising difficulty observers have in noticing large changes to visual scenes."
(Simons & Rensink, 2005)

Change blindness is a failure to detect that an object has moved or disappeared and is the opposite of change detection. The phenomenon of change blindness can be demonstrated even when the change in question is large. Often we can't see large changes even though we think they would be easy to notice. If we aren't observing certain things we don't often notice them. One experiment I read about online was a simple thing that a teacher does every year to his freshman science class; he asks everyone to look around the room and observe things in the class room then takes everyone out into the hall and asks them question on what they saw. Once the students can an idea of what they are looking for he then brings them back into the room after he has switched a few things and asks them what is now different. The students couldn't seem to pick out the small subtle changes he made to the room such as switching posters or books but they also didn't seem to notice the large changes he made such as switching desks or chairs. Now in some of my research it stated that this is an example of how everyone experiences change blindness in some forms.

When asking how change blindness relates to chapter 7 the answer is pretty simple. Chapter 7 discusses how we often miss things; up until now we have focused on what we see and how we process what we see. Chapter 7 introduces us to the fact that sometimes we miss things and don't see what we should be seeing and this error in our sight is change blindness. One thing that I found interesting with change blindness was that although it is something we all experience it tends to be worse with older adults. As we age it is harder for us to notice certain changes and be able to respond to them. Along with that the older people had problems with the studies I read about all together; not only could they not notice the change but they also struggled with looking at the pictures for differences.


One study I found very intersting was the one by O’Regan, Rensink, & Clarke done in 1999 they found that when small shapes are splattered over an image, such as mudsplashes over a car windshield, large changes can be made to a visual scene without the observer noticing. While previous research demonstrated that change blindness could be produced by a visual disruption such as flickering, blinking, or eye movement, this study demonstrated that change blindness can also occur without visual masking. I found this study interesting because the definition of change blindness kind of lead me to believe that it is the flash in between pictures or the time when you aren't seeing the image that makes it hard to notice the change but this study showed that people even when consistently looking at an image sometimes can tell the difference between them.

The next basic question about change blindness that I had was "why does this happen?" so far in the course we have learned that our eyes are amazing and can do some much. That is why I liked focusing on this topic because it seemed like a topic that was a flaw that all of us tended to have. It is something that is unavoidable. So why does this happen? In my research I found that this happens because of attention; which was the main focus of chapter 7. We can only place our full attention on one thing at a time, when we are paying attention to something (for example I am paying attention to typing what I am thinking) we often let large things go by without being noticed (like I may not notice the cars driving by outside due to my attention being on my typing). This is a really interesting topic to me because we can't change this, we can't force ourselves to see more or less or pay attention to more or less than we did before. Basically a lot of information passes us by because we lack the resources to attend to it; because our attention is often focused elsewhere.

Terms used: Change blindness, sensation, perception, inattentional blindness, attention, visual system, visual field.

http://www.cogsci.uci.edu/~ddhoff/cb.html this website gave more information and examples of change blindness that was helpful in me applying the concept to myself.

http://psychology.about.com/od/cognitivepsychology/f/change-blindness.htm I liked this source because it shared a lot of research studies that have been done and it was interesting to read about them. Reading about these made me want to research more studies that have been done on the subject.


http://psychology.about.com/od/cognitivepsychology/f/change-blindness.htm this website gave examples of what change blindness can be defined as and how it is something that we all experience.

For this week’s assignment, I chose to do more research on Balint Syndrome. Balint Syndrome is defined as an uncommon and incompletely understood triad of severe neuropsychological impairments: inability to perceive the visual field as a whole (simultanagnosia), difficulty in fixating the eyes (oculomotor apraxia), and inability to move the hand to a specific object by using vision (optic ataxia). This topic fits with our current chapter and also with the course because it is yet another disorder than can affect our sensation and/or perception of the world around us.

Starting this research and trying to find just a specific definition of this syndrome was quite exhausting. Through the websites I found and read the definitions, basically the part that caught my eye was simultagnosia, the inability to take in the entirety of a picture while instead seeing only parts of the whole. For example, when shown a picture of a house, someone with simultagnosia could only see a window, a door, a wall, and so on, but not the entire house. The disorder usually results from damage to both parietal lobes, which help us know where we, and other objects, are in space. When symptoms come on suddenly, they're likely due to stroke; but other disorders such as tumors, and even neurodegenerative diseases such as Alzheimer’s, can also sometimes lead to Balint’s syndrome. Someone with Balint’s syndrome can’t judge her location relative to things around her by sight: she must depend on other senses to guide her. She may, for example, need to keep a hand on the sink in order to know where it is in the bathroom. And she may need to put toothpaste into her mouth, rather than on the toothbrush. She may not be able to use utensils at the table, since she can’t use vision to aim her hand to pick up a fork or a spoon. It may be impossible for people with Balint’s syndrome to read, since simultagnosia means she may only see one letter at a time, and may not be able to put that letter into the context of a word or sentence.

On a positive note, there are some therapies out there to help with this syndrome. Occupational therapy offers some means for people with Balint’s syndrome to recover a degree of independence. While different approaches have been suggested, no one approach is clearly the best, and it may be helpful to consider using techniques normally used to assist the blind. For example, therapists may suggest ways to use other senses to replace damaged perception. Books on tape can be used instead of usual reading material, and a radio may replace television viewing. In order to get this kind of assistance, though, both the patient and the medical professional must be aware of the problem in the first place. Because Balint’s syndrome is relatively uncommon, the symptoms are often missed. Knowing that visual disturbances may not always be a problem with the eyes, but may also result from trouble with the brain, is a good start.

References:

http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Bálint's_syndrome I chose this website because I searched many other sites for just a definition of Balint’s Syndrome. I never came across a formal or concrete definition. I know Wikipedia is user updated, but this website was a definition a lay person could somewhat understand.

http://psy2.fau.edu/~rosselli/NeuroLab/pdfs/rehabilitation.pdf I chose this website because it had a case study report in it. There was also more therapies listed in this article.

https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=4odhSq46vtU I chose to include a YouTube video of this syndrome because many of us who have read about this topic, probably are curious (as I was) to see what this was really like for someone.

TERMS: Balint Syndrome, visual field, simultanagnosia, oculomotor apraxia, optic ataxia, parietal lobe, neurodegenerative diseases, perception, sensation,

iv decided to do change blindness. this is a interesting concept one that we don't notice very much unless we are looking for it. change blindness has to do with when we are involved in something and there is a object that changes and we don't seem to notice. of course there are those of us that do notice that something has changed and point it out to people. then we feel stupid for not realizing it has happened when we had a sense something was different. the thing about this where I notice it a lot is movies and TV shows. I notice it here a lot because I am a movie person and like to have the details of the show. other people may notice it in every day life. people will randomly change a shirt at work or school and people may not notice but feel something is different. It is a change that is not major in our lives so we don't give it our full attention. I find this very interesting because it makes me think about all the other small things in life that we might not notice. there is a video that i found that discribs change blindness and gives a experiment of it as well. we not only have to make sure that our eyes are doing the work but to make sure that our brain is following along. because there is so much information that our eyes are picking up the brain cant processes it all. so if its not important information our brain chooses to ignore it.

change blindness, brain, eyes, information,
http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Change_blindness

https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=Qb-gT6vDrmU

http://psychology.about.com/od/cognitivepsychology/f/change-blindness.htm

1a) Balint syndrome and simultagnosia

1b) Balint syndrome and simultagnosia were vocabulary terms from chapter 7 and are brought up in a few paragraphs.

1c) Balint syndrome and simultagnosia was interesting just because I tried to imagine what a person with balint syndrome might view the world. Humans naturally are curious about things that are different than what they have, so I am very interested in this abnormality from the chapter.

2) Balint syndrome happens when a person has bilateral lesions of the partial lobes within the brain. It is an odd condition and does not happen very often. Balint syndrome has three major symptoms. The first is spatial localization abilities are greatly reduced. This results in a patient having a very hard time trying to reach toward on object near them or grab something when it is trying to be handed to them. The second symptom is patients with this disorder do not move their eyes as much as a person without does. They tend to have a more fixed gaze. The last symptom of balint syndrome is that they behave as though they can only see one object at a time. This deficit seems like an extreme form of extinction or neglect that when focusing on one object, all other surrounding objects are eliminated in doing so. The inability to perceive more than one object at a time is referred to as simultagnosia.

Balint's syndrome occurs most often with a severe onset as a consequence of two or more strokes at more or less the same place in each hemisphere of the brain. Therefore, it occurs rarely but makes it harder for us to really study and understand it further. The most frequent cause of complete balint's syndrome is said by some to be sudden and severe hypotension. This then results in bilateral border zone infarction in the occipital-parietal region. In even more rare cases of progressive balint's syndrome have been found in degenerative disorders such as Alzheimer's disease or certain other traumatic brain injuries at the border of the parietal and the occipital lobes of the brain.
Lack of awareness of the syndrome may lead to misdiagnosis of the disease and is often diagnosed in different disorders such as blindness, psychosis, or dementia. Symptoms of balint's syndrome are most likely to be noticed first by therapists providing rehabilitation following brain lesions and surgery. This becomes a problem due to the shortage of practitioners that have familiarity with the syndrome. The symptoms are often explained away incorrectly without being considered as a possibility and followed by medical confirmation of clinical and neurological findings.

As far as treatment options for balint syndrome there are three approaches to rehabilitation of perceptual deficits that are used with other similar disorders. The first is the adaptive approach. The adaptive approach involves functional tasks utilizing the person’s current strengths and abilities. This helps them to compensate for problems or altering the environment to lessen their disabilities. This is the most popular approach for balint syndrome. Another approach is the remedial approach. In this approach, restoration of the damaged CNS is fixed by training in the perceptual skills. This may be generalized across all activities of daily living. This could be achieved by tabletop activities or sensorimotor exercises. The final approach is the multi-context approach. This approach is based on the fact that learning is not automatically transferred from one situation to another. This involves practicing of a targeted strategy in a multiple environment with varied tasks and movement demands, and it incorporates self-awareness tasks. When dealing with an actual balint syndrome diagnosed patient they may use a combination of these three approaches to try and correct the disabilities though not very much success has been shown yet. While they cannot completely fix the syndrome, they can still train other parts of the brain to function better in order to complete everyday tasks that are difficult to do with this disease. Most of the information that is currently available is from case studies on balint syndrome.

Simultagnosia, as stated previously, is a rare neurological disorder characterized by the inability of an individual to perceive more than a single object at a time. This type of visual attention problem is one of three major components of balint syndrome. Simultagnosia can be divided into two different categories dorsal and ventral. Ventral occipital-temporal lesions cause a mild form of the disorder. Ventral results from damage to the left inferior occipital-temporal junction. Ventral simultagnosia patients are able to see several objects at once, but their recognition of objects is limited to one object at a time. Thus, individuals with ventral simultagnosia symptoms are capable of navigating through a room without bumping into things. Dorsal occipital-parietal lesions cause a more severe form of the disorder. This is when there are bilateral lesions to the junction between the parietal and occipital lobes. With dorsal perception is limited to a single object without awareness of the presence of other stimuli. This can cause a patient to collide with various objects in a room being unaware of them. Also, objects in motion appear more difficult to see.

Terms: Balint syndrome, simultagnosia, ventral, dorsal, occipital-parietal lobes, motion, rehabilitation, adaptive approach, remedial approach, multi-context approach

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC1737281/pdf/v070p00416.pdf This is a section of an article going over the different treatment options currently available for balint syndrome.

http://vectors.usc.edu/issues/4/malperception/simultanagnosia.html this site explains in better detail dorsal simultagnosia and even has an activity to try to show what it may be like to have it.

http://informahealthcare.com/doi/abs/10.3109/02699052.2011.585506 This is a case report on balint syndrome that goes into rehabilitation and symptoms of the disorder

http://www.medscape.com/viewarticle/410860_5 This article goes into more depth on the types of simultagnosia along with a better explanation of what causes the disorder.

1. The topic I decided to explore is multitasking. It relates to chapter 7 as this chapter covered the different types of attention. This led me to think about multitasking as people do it all the time.
2. Multitasking is defined as “the apparent performance by an individual of handling more than one task, or activity, at the same time”, according to Wikipedia. If you ask most people, they would say that they are good at multitasking, and that they do it a lot. They would be wrong about one thing, because most people are not good multitaskers as only 2% of people can accurately call themselves “:good multitaskers”. According to one study, 70% of people thought they were “above average multitaskers” which we know is statistically impossible. Not only are people not good at multitasking, just the simple act of multitasking hurts people in a few different ways.
One reason people are not good multitaskers is that working memory capacity of the brain is not designed to work like that, which negatively impacts it ability to do several cognitive processes. Both analytic and creative problem solving skills decrease as the number of tasks increases. This is because there is not enough focus being put into the each task. This results in the performer not being able to “get in the zone” because they are constantly switching from one task to another (also known as context switch). This is called Continuous partial attention. This explains why continuous partial attention decrease productivity by 40%, it has also said that is can decrease a person’s IQ by ten whole points (which is the same effect as losing one night of sleep) while they are distracted-both of these slow down the average multitasker. The age of the performer also impacts the ability to multitask; younger people’s brains are able to get back on track to a revisited task faster than older brains.
Not only does multitasking hurt the brain of the performer, it hurts other aspects of life. Multitasking can increase stress levels, as one study proved when they measured the heart rates of employees constantly checking their emails and compared them to the heart rates of employees without access to their emails. The employees who could check inboxes had higher heart rates than the other group; this was because the first group was in a “perpetual high alert mode”. This is an example of how too much stress negatively impacts performance, and increases the chances of making mistakes. Multitasking during meals can also cause a person to over eat when they would not normally do so. This happens because the brain is too distracted to accurately measure when the stomach is actually full.
While both of these effects aren’t immediately going to cause negative side effects, there is one common multitasking situation can do just that. People using cellphones, texting or otherwise, while driving should not be done. The reasoning behind that is that the drivers’ reaction time decreases due to their divided attention as much as a blood alcohol count of .08, and this was the result of using both hands-free technology and handheld technology. One study even said that 1in 5 teens who had to go to the ER after being in a car accident eventually fessed up to using a cell phone at the time of the car accident. This level of distraction also impacts a person’s safety while walking, as people tend to be more likely to cross traffic lanes without checking to make sure they were not going to be hit by any cars.
Even with all of that being said, there is sometimes when multitasking is okay. I discovered this because I wondered if listen to music while doing a homework assignment counted as multitasking, and for the record it does. Music can have a positive impact on people as they do repetitive tasks. “Up beat music” as the source said, can help factory workers stay focused on boring, repeated tasks. Up beat music can also help a person’s ability to complex cognitive tasks, and tasks that require creativity but only if the music is listened to before the tasks are completed. And if music is heard, it would be better for it to “low information-load” music. Classical, nature sounds, and word-less music is examples of music best used when no background sound would be detrimental to the complex and creative task being done.
3. Terms: multitasking, attention, working memory capacity, brain , cognitive processes, analytic, creative problem solving skills , tasks, focus, Continuous partial attention, productivity, IQ, stress levels, heart rates , reaction time , blood alcohol count , “low information-load” music.

4. http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Human_multitasking I picked this page because it clearly explained all about human multitasking. I used it for: definition, continuous partial attention, IQ
http://www.health.com/health/gallery/0,,20707868,00.html I picked this site because it gave me lots of condensed, easy to understand info to work with. I used it for: “get in the zone”, age of brain, stress levels, eating and multitasking, texting and driving
http://mashable.com/2012/08/13/multitasking-infographic/ I picked this site because it clearly explained the stats behind multitasking. I used it for the stats, IQ info, driving and texting
http://cdp.sagepub.com/content/21/4/258.short I picked this article because it explained the mental side of multitasking. I used for the bit on working memory capacity, attentional focus, and problem solving.
http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Context_switch I picked this site because it was linked to the other Wikipedia page. I used it for the term “context switch”.
http://unews.utah.edu/news_releases/frequent-mulitaskers-are-bad-at-it/ I picked this study because it clearly explained the results. I used it for: 70% stat, talking while driving.
https://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/conquering-cyber-overload/201305/is-background-music-boost-or-bummer I picked this study because it explained how music can both help and hinder a person when being done.

I chose the topic of inattentional blindness because I find it so fascinating that as a parent I can spot lie from a teenager a mile away but miss the most obvious of things in a video or television program. I think I am particularly susceptible to intentional blindness. I am constantly missing things and my husband will ask if I saw a person at a function and I will say no and he is flabbergasted because the person was there and I walked right by her. This type of scenario happens frequently. I wondered if there was a connection between this and ADD but I was not able to find very much that would support a connection. Perhaps it happens more frequently? I am not sure.
What I did find is that inattentional blindness serves a purpose. It helps us when we are focusing on something so that we are not distracted by all that is going on around us. This phenomenon is when we don’t see something very obvious that does not fit with a particular scene that we are concentrating on at the moment. The term was coined by two psychologists, Daniel Simons and Christopher Chabris at Harvard in the early 1990’s. Their research is based on the study of selective looking procedure that was introduced by psychologist Ulric Neisser at Cornell University. Some researchers have speculated that this is a form of amnesia that we in fact recognize the oddity but simply forget it very quickly. Although it is still up for debate, most of the information I read suggests that the majority of researchers believe that we simply don’t observe some unexpected things when our focus is fixated on a specific task. It is important to understand this because it helps us to realize that although we can be at the location of an event there is the possibility we could miss something very obvious.
One real world involved a very serious case with the Boston Police department in 1995. The officers received a call of a officer who had been shot. A car chase ensues and eventually several officers leave their cars to chase the suspect on foot. Two officers mistake an undercover officer for the suspect in the darkness and begin to beat him. As this is occurring anther officer named Conley ran past in pursuit of the suspect and didn’t stop to help the undercover officer. Conley’s claim was that he was so focused on the pursuit he did not notice the two officers beating the undercover officer. Conley admitted that he had been right in the area but that he had not seen anything. He repeated over and over that he couldn’t believe he didn’t see the commotion and that he wished he had. As it turns out no one else could believe it either and he was convicted of perjury and lost his position on the force. It was eventually overturned but because the state withheld evidence and he had already lost his job. Since that time there have been several studies and tests that show how we can so easily miss the obvious.
I watched several videos that tested the inattentional blindness. I did see the inserted object in one case but missed it in the others. In my very first Intro to psych class I missed the gorilla on the basketball court. I’ve watched it several times since and each time I am so flabbergasted that I ever missed the gorilla the first time, it now seems so obvious. However, I did find that I am not alone. Inattentive blindness happens to all of us, no matter our intelligence. This has been tested on airline pilots who missed things brought in on screens when they were focusing on other issues. A group of twenty-four radiologists who were given 239 frames of a chest x-ray and told to identify cancer in them. Five frames in a row contained an image of a gorilla. Although they are highly trained to examine the images twenty of the radiologists did not see the image of the gorilla. The researchers used eye tracking devices that showed that the doctors had looked right at the gorilla. So although they can regularly detect cancer modules that are much smaller than the added image they did not see the gorilla because they simply weren’t looking for it and didn’t expect it.

Experts believe the reason we are so surprised by inattentional blindness is because for the most part we don’t encounter it in our daily lives. In most cases the world is pretty stable, what we expect should be in a location is and there are few unexpected surprises. For instance we rarely have a gorilla running out on a basketball court, or a picture of one on an x-ray of a lung.
Terms: inattentional blindness, observe, focus, perceive, observe

https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=wdVXco6YDgg
Video of the radiologists
http://www.apa.org/monitor/apr01/blindness.aspx
Explanation, history and examples of inattentional blindness
http://www.scholarpedia.org/article/Inattentional_blindness
Another page with information on inattentional blindness and some other explanations
http://www.npr.org/2011/06/20/137086464/why-seeing-the-unexpected-is-often-not-believing
Story of the Boston Police Officer
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=IZq-5i9PetE
The video where I did see the spider

The concept of time to collision from chapter 7 was interesting to me,so that is my topic. I've been in a boat load of car accidents, more than the national average I think, so collision to me is second nature. Also my lack of hand-eye coordination when it comes to connecting with a soccer ball or tennis ball seems to fit into the subject.

Speaking of hand-eye coordination, estimating time to collision can also help an organism achieve a desired collision, like hitting an object with a body part or another object. This can be beneficial in a variety of settings, like kicking a goal in soccer, or hitting a tennis ball where you need it. Turns out people are actually pretty good at this sort of thing, better at judging time to collision than would be expected-well, some are better than others, that goes without saying, but I said it anyway. There is an alternative source of information in the optic flow that could signal TTC without the necessity of estimating either absolute distances or rates, known as tau. An example of how tau works is a ball coming toward one’s face, and the image on one’s retina growing larger as it approaches. The ratio of the retinal image size at any moment to the rate at which the image is expanding is tau, and TTC is proportional to tau. An advantage of using tau to estimate TTC is that it relies solely on information available directly from the retinal image.

The survival of many animals hinges upon their ability to avoid collisions with other animals or objects, or to precisely control the timing of collisions. Optical expansion provides a compelling impression of an object’s approach. In principle, this can provide the basis for judgments of time to collision. Some animals even have neural systems that can initiate rapid coordinated actions on the basis of optical expansion. But in humans, the link between judgments of TTC and coordinated action has not been established at a cortical level, so despite all that, were still alive, so must mean were not too bad at this collision stuff.

It can also be translated into everyday use of a vehicle. Back-up cameras and warning systems installed in new vehicles allow drivers to see how close they are getting to another object, how much time they have before needing to change directions, or being warned if their car has gotten unsafely close to another object. According to the European Commission for Safety, the reaction time to an accident or impending accident is about 1 second, within that second the warning and assistance phase which features optic, acoustic and/or haptic warnings goes off. The warning even transforms into active assistance under one second. In a haptic warning system, the active accelerator starts to develop an increasingly stronger force during the warning phase, indicating the need for corrective measures. This is followed by breaking assistance, and finally by autonomous braking in order to prevent the vehicle from running over a pedestrian or hitting another car. Considering my record this stuff is a must in my next car.This new feature can prevent, or lessen the damage of a collision. These new features serve as the time to collision abilities our human visual system has, but now our cars can utilize these abilities to protect us while driving. Because not all of us are fully conscious, or fully aware of everything that goes on around us while we drive. I think it’s interesting that humans have continued to adapt the tools we use to make our lives easier and more efficient, by integrating the biological abilities we possess into the tools we use. With these time to collision warning systems, our cars will help us to do what our visual system has always done to protect our bodies.

Sources:
http://pdn.sciencedirect.com/science?_ob=MiamiImageURL&_cid=272099&_user=10&_pii=S0960982205001661&_check=y&_coverDate=2005-03-08&view=c&_gw=y&wchp=dGLbVBA-zSkzV&md5=772ee3f8ab17c2202cd3eabb950c2832/1-s2.0-S0960982205001661-main.pdf
I actually only read the results from this file, cuts straight to the point.
http://pdn.sciencedirect.com/science?_ob=MiamiImageURL&_cid=272099&_user=10&_pii=S0960982200004930&_check=y&_coverDate=2000-05-15&view=c&_gw=y&wchp=dGLzVlt-zSkzk&md5=d32cc26d2d1cc73f3c8ca040221c2467/1-s2.0-S0960982200004930-main.pdf
Its the same thing with this link, I looked at the abstract, then went to the discussion and results. It makes a lot of sense and the diagrams help give you a visual.
http://www.autoevolution.com/news/braking-systems-history-6933.html
This link contains the history of breaking systems in cars, shows how far the tech has come.

(1a) Change Blindness

(1b) Change blindness relates to the chapter because the book discusses how it is hard for us to tell differences or change in two images or two scenes.

(1c) I was interested in this topic because I am so focused on calling myself a good multi-tasker, which in turn this chapter provides information to tell me that my attention does not have the span or ability to be a good multi-tasker. I can do many tasks at one time, but me remembering what I did and paying attention to those different tasks may be difficult.

(2) Change blindness is a perceptual phenomenon that occurs when a change in a visual stimulus is introduced and the observer does not notice it once so ever. Human attention has its limitations, one of these limitations is that people have a poor ability to detect changes. Change blindness has become a highly researched topic of interest, some even say it could provided important implications in eyewitness testimony and distractions while driving. Research today has been focusing on an abundance of topics, change detection was one of them. Individuals notice a change faster when required to detect changes in facial features than when required to detect changes in images of houses. Mental processing in change blindness relates to the increased brain activity in the parietal- occipital regions prior to the emergence of a change in a change blindness task. Another topic of research related to lucid dreaming, lucid dreaming occurs when one realizes that the events experienced within a dream are bizarre and would not occur in one's waking life. Inability to notice the bizarre nature of this dream is coined as an example of change blindness. Change blindness in teams has also been researched, teams working together notice changes in an image when assistance is present.

The detail dispersed among us when we look out at the world is extremely vast and available to us. But of the vast amount of detail, you would be surprised that we actually remember very little about a scene that we have just looked at. Ron Rensink and his colleagues investigate this phenomenon using a flicker paradigm, observers see a visual scene that flickers on and off. During this flicker scene, something of some sort in the scene changes. An object may move and reposition itself to another position, the object may change color, or it could just vanish. Experimenters like Rensink have found that participants are very poor at detecting these obvious changes. Detecting changes in a scene for example is hard to notice, magicians use sleight of hand to distract our attention away somewhere else while they perform a card trick. The flicker paradigm seems to be the best of the best when analyzing change blindness, the reason being is because it provides quick feedback and implementing the procedure is so easy and understandable.

The role of attention and how it operates in neuronal processing is a topic that has gone unresolved. The main focus of attention in individuals is to maintain a very minimum degree of visual awareness with which they can detect and discriminate classes of information at little or no cost to the central attentional processing circuit. What was found in a study was that attention tends to pool opposite of a cued location. Attention then is said to be object centered compared to location centered since visual awareness and attention is pooled away due to the smallest amount of peripheral cue obstruction. Experimenters expressed that mapping out attention in regards to studies done in change blindness will provide the best information.

(3) Terms : change blindness, attention, cue, parietal lobe, selective attention, visual awareness, flicker paradigm, change detection

(4) http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Change_blindness
This source has provided me with information I used to present a defined and accurate definition of change blindness and the research being done now a days to examine it.

http://nivea.psycho.univ-paris5.fr/ECS/ECS-CB.html
This source provided credible information that I used to describe experimental work done in regards to change blindness. I used Rensink and his flicker paradigm because it is the easiest to implement, and understand. While it also provided the most accurate results

http://mapageweb.umontreal.ca/gosselif/labogo/tse.pdf
This source provided information that I used to show the importance of mapping out visual attention with change blindness. This will provide the best information in regards to this new method of direction in future studies.

Leave a comment

Recent Entries

Week #14 Online-Line assignment (Due Thursday)
You should already have a product or service picked out for your final. Please do some in-depth research on the…
Week #13 Online Assignment - Extra credit (Due Monday)
Topics in the News?What I would like you to do is to start applying what we are learning in class to…
Week 12 Online assignment (Due Monday)
Please go to the following site, poke around and find something interesting to you and write about it from a…